Publications
Reprint Requests
Empirical dissociations between rule-based and similarity-based categorization.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 28(1), 15-16.
(2005). Encoding models in neuroimaging.
In F. G. Ashby, H. Colonius, & E. Dzhafarov (Eds.), The new handbook of mathematical psychology, Volume 3 (pp. 421-472). Cambridge University Press.
(2023). Erasing the engram: The unlearning of procedural skills.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 142(3), 710-741.
(2013). Estimating the parameters of multidimensional signal detection theory from simultaneous ratings on separate stimulus components.
Perception & Psychophysics. 44, 195–204.
(1988). Evidence for cortical automaticity in rule-based categorization.
Journal of Neuroscience. 30(42), 14225-14234.
(2010). Expanding the role of striatal cholinergic interneurons and the midbrain dopamine system in appetitive instrumental conditioning.
Journal of Neurophysiology. 115(1), 240-54.
(2016). Experimental test of contemporary mathematical models of visual letter recognition..
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 8, 834.
(1982). Fitting computational models to fMRI.
Behavioral Research Methods. 40(3), 713-721.
(2008).
(2003). A formal theory of feature binding in object perception.
Psychological Review. 103, 165-192.
(1996). FROST: a distributed neurocomputational model of working memory maintenance.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 17(11), 1728-1743.
(2005). A general recognition theory model for identifying an ideal stimulus.
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. 84, 2408–2421.
(2022). General recognition theory with individual differences: a new method for examining perceptual and decisional interactions with an application to face perception.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 22(1), 88-111.
(2015). Generalization of category knowledge and dimensional categorization in humans (Homo sapiens) and nonhuman primates (Macaca mulatta).
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning & Cognition. 41(4), 322-335.
(2015). Hierarchical control of procedural and declarative category-learning systems.
NeuroImage. 150, 150-161.
(2017). Human category learning.
Annual Review of Psychology. 56, 149-178.
(2005). Human category learning 2.0.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1224, 147-161.
(2011). Human category learning, neural basis.
The encyclopedia of the mind. 130–134.
(2013). The impact of category separation on unsupervised categorization.
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. 74(2), 466-475.
(2012). Implicit and explicit categorization: A tale of four species.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 36(10), 2355-2369.
(2012). Implicit and explicit category learning by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
Journal of Comparative Psychology. 126(3), 294-304.
(2012). Implicit and explicit category learning by macaques (Macaca mulatta) and humans (Homo sapiens).
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 36(1), 54-65.
(2010). Increased cognitive load enables unlearning in procedural category learning.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition. 44, 1845-1853.
(2018). Influence of positive affect on the subjective utility of gains and losses: It's just not worth the risk..
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 55, 710-717.
(1988). Information-integration category learning and the human uncertainty response.
Memory & Cognition. 39(3), 536-554.
(2011).